Implicit and Explicit Memory: Definition & Examples

Implicit memory is unconscious recall, like skills and habits (e.g., riding a bike), while explicit memory is conscious recall of facts and events (e.g., remembering a birthday). Both are vital components of long-term memory, with implicit being more about “knowing how” and explicit about “knowing that.”

Key Takeaways

  • Implicit and explicit memories represent the distinct neural processes and the different states of awareness of our long-term memory.
  • Explicit memory involves recalling previously learned information that requires conscious effort to receive, while implicit memory is unconscious and effortless.
  • Explicit memory fades in the absence of recall, while implicit memory is more robust and may last a lifetime, even without further practice.
  • The discovery of implicit and explicit memory stemmed from treating a patient suffering from amnesia. Typically, amnesic patients have great difficulty retaining episodic and semantic information following the onset of amnesia.
  • While implicit memory involves perceptional and emotional unconscious memories, explicit memory involves information and experiences we can consciously recall.
  • Despite much research and studies, the exact nature of the relationship between implicit and explicit memory is still ambiguous.

Our long-term memory can be fundamentally divided into two distinct types, namely implicit memory and explicit memory (Squire, 2004).

It should be noted that the formation of explicit memories requires several rounds of stimulation, significant effort, and considerable time. Alternatively, a single stimulus may trigger the learning and retention of implicit memories.

Moreover, while implicit memory relies on specified areas of the brain, explicit memory depends upon multicomponent brain links involving the brain’s cortical and temporal regions.

Differences between implicit and explicit memory

Origin and Development

The discovery of implicit and explicit memory stemmed from the treatment of the neuroscience patient, Henry Gustav Molaison (Squire, 2009).

An attempt to cure his epilepsy via a bilateral medial temporal lobotomy destroyed parts of Molaison’s brain. Consequently, he suffered from amnesia. Although following the surgery, Molaison was able to form short-term memories, his long-term memory was impaired.

Molaison was able to quickly learn skills such as hand-to-eye coordination. However, he could not recall events from his former days prior to the surgery.

Memory for events and knowledge acquired before the onset of amnesia tend to remain intact, but amnesiacs can’t store new episodic or semantic memories.

In other words, it appears that their ability to retain declarative information is impaired.

However, their procedural memory appears to be largely unaffected. They can recall skills they have already learned (e.g., riding a bike) and acquire new skills (e.g., learning to drive).

The nature of Molaison’s amnesia provided scientists insight into the workings of different memory systems as well as the brain structures governing their functioning.

In addition to Molaison’s case, the study of patients suffering from various forms of neurodegeneration and trauma too, has developed our understanding of implicit and explicit memory (Squire, 2015).

For instance, examining how the damaged hippocampus of patients with Alzheimer’s disease impacts their ability to create and retain explicit memories has generated important discussion.

What is Implicit Memory?

Implicit memory, also known as unconscious memory or automatic memory, refers to perceptional and emotional unconscious memories which influence our behavior (Dew & Cabeza, 2011).

The impact of implicit memory on our current behavior occurs without conscious retrieval of memories.

Hence, implicit memory enables our prior experiences to improve our performance of various tasks without our conscious and explicit awareness of such experiences.

Types of Implicit Memory

Procedural Learning

  • Procedural memory is part of implicit memory that is responsible for knowing how to perform a particular type of action, such as reading, tying shoes, and riding a bike.
  • Procedural memories are automatically retrieved for the execution of procedures involved in both cognitive and motor skills. This enables task performance without the need for conscious control or attention.
  • The association of procedural learning with muscle memory can make certain actions second nature (Bullemer, Nissen, & Willingham, 1989).

Priming

  • Priming is a non-conscious form of human implicit memory concerned with the perceptual identification of words and objects. Priming can be associative, negative, positive, affective, conceptual, perceptual, repetitive, or semantic.
  • The subtle effects which this complex psychological phenomenon encompasses can be employed to manipulate individual behavior.

Category Learning

  •  Category learning involves the attainment of a concept in order to clarify and categorize various entities via grouping (Ell, Shawn, Zilioli, & Monica, 2012). Category learning allows for comparisons and indicates subjective divisions for better comprehension.

Perceptual Learning

  • Perceptual learning constitutes the foundation for cognitive processes and cooperates with the neural basis to produce the prime effect. Perceptual learning also improves perception by enabling the distinguishing of similar things from each other.

Emotional Learning

  • Emotional learning, which involves autobiographical memories entangled with emotions, refers to the impact of emotions upon an individual.

Examples of Implicit Memory

Some examples of implicit memory include knowing how to play the piano, ride a bike, tie your shoes, and other motor skills. These skills involve procedural knowledge, which involves “knowing how” to do things.

Other examples of implicit memory may include:

    • Knowing how to make breakfast.
    • Knowing how to play a musical instrument.
    • Navigating a familiar area such as your house or neighborhood.

Skills using implicit memory do not involve conscious thought (i.e., they are unconscious and automatic). For example, we brush our teeth with little or no awareness of the skills involved.

Related Brain Structures

The functioning of implicit memory is thought to involve the cerebellum and the basal ganglia (Dew & Cabeza, 2011). The cerebellum, which is essential for procedural memories, is located at the base of the brain.

Although it does not initiate actions, the cerebellum receives and coordinates signals from the spinal cord, the brain, and sensory systems to carry out motor movements. & Cabeza, 2011). The cerebellum, which is essential for procedural memories, is located at the base of the brain.

The cerebellum, which forms the hindbrain, is primarily responsible for skill development as well as a few cognitive tasks such as attention and language (13.2 The Central Nervous System – Anatomy and Physiology, 2013). & Cabeza, 2011). The cerebellum, which is essential for procedural memories, is located at the base of the brain.

The basal ganglia, on the other hand, which engage in action selection, are essential for the smooth controlling of sequential movements (Ullman, 2004).

Responsible for processes such as habit formation and the regulation of emotions, the basal ganglia consist of a pair of structures deep inside the brain. & Cabeza, 2011). The cerebellum, which is essential for procedural memories, is located at the base of the brain.

The basal ganglia’s constitution explains why implicit memory involves subconsciously driven sensorimotor behavior, which we typically remain unaware of.

What is Explicit Memory?

Explicit memory, also known as declarative memory, refers to memories involving personal experiences as well as factual information which we can consciously retrieve and intentionally articulate (Dew & Cabeza, 2011).

Explicit memory has to do with remembering who, what, where, when, and why.

Recalling information from explicit memory involves conscious effort – information is consciously brought to mind and “declared.”

For example, declarative knowledge involves “knowing that” London is the capital of England, zebras are animals, and the date of your mom’s birthday, etc. (Cohen & Squire, 1980).

Types of Explicit Memory

Semantic Memory

  • Semantic memory is a type of explicit memory that includes your areas of expertise, general academic knowledge, and everyday knowledge about the meanings of words and common things. 
  • Semantic memory is a part of the long-term memory responsible for storing information about the world. This includes knowledge about the meaning of words and general knowledge.
  • For example, London is the capital of England. It involves conscious thought and is declarative.

Autobiographical Memory

  • Autobiographical memory involves various episodes from the past gathered from our personal history based on a certain time, space, object, or person. Autobiographical memories often combine episodic and semantic memories.

Episodic Memory

  • Episodic memory is a type of long-term memory responsible for storing information about events (i.e., episodes) we have experienced.
  • Episodic memories involve conscious thought and can be declared explicitly. An example would be a memory of our 1st day at school.

Spatial Memory

  •  Spatial memory is crucial for the formation of cognitive maps. It involves the recording of facts concerning an individual’s spatial arrangement. Spatial memory accounts for our readily finding our way through familiar towns.

Examples of Explicit Memory

Our knowledge in semantic and episodic memories focuses on “knowing that” something is the case (i.e., declarative).

For example, we might have a semantic memory for knowing that Paris is the capital of France, and we might have an episodic memory for knowing that we caught the bus to college today.

Other examples of explicit memory may include:

    • Recollecting the items on a to-do list.
    • Remembering the dates of various events for a history exam.
    • Remembering the time for a doctor’s appointment.

Related Brain Structures

Explicit memory is governed by communications between the prefrontal cortex, the amygdala, and the hippocampus (Dew & Cabeza, 2011).

The prefrontal cortex is thought to be necessary to store and retrieve long-term memories involving information and facts (13.2 The Central Nervous System – Anatomy and Physiology, 2013).

Located deep within the brain’s temporal lobe, the hippocampus is essential for spatial awareness and navigation and the consolidation of information from short-term to long-term memory (Squire, 2015).

The hippocampus does not involve implicit memory. The amygdala, which engages in emotional learning, is located near the hippocampus.

While the retention and the recalling of events rely on the function of the hippocampus, the declarative inside the brain’s medial temporal lobe is consolidated into the temporal cortex (Squire, 2009).

The Relationship between the Two Memory Systems

While recent evidence suggests a significant impact of implicit memory’s priming on explicit memory’s fact recalling, the two memory systems are thought to work independently with fundamentally distinct rules of operation (Squire, 2004).

The study of amnesic patients implies a separation of implicit and explicit memory. For instance, on one occasion, some amnesic patients with severely impaired long-term verbal memories demonstrated no difficulty mastering a certain puzzle even though they could not recall having seen it before (Brooks & Baddeley, 1976).

While damage to the hippocampus may explain the loss of explicit memory, the loss of their ability for conscious remembering seems to have still left intact various residual learning abilities.

Despite this apparent separation, implicit and explicit memory seems to work parallel to shape our behavior (Squire, 2015).

The relationship between the two memory systems may also be influenced by chronic drug use, aging, and stress. Despite manifold studies and much research, however, the exact nature of this relationship is still ambiguous (Dew & Cabeza, 2011).

Consequently, whether the two memory systems are cooperating with or competing against each other is yet to be discovered.

FAQs

How is an explicit memory different from an implicit memory?

Explicit memory is conscious and intentional retrieval of facts, events, or personal experiences. It involves conscious awareness and effortful recollection, such as recalling specific details of a past event or remembering facts from a textbook.

In contrast, implicit memory is unconscious and automatic memory processing without conscious awareness. It includes skills, habits, and priming effects, where past experiences influence behavior or cognitive processes without conscious effort or awareness.,

Which part of the brain is most involved in creating implicit memories?

The part of the brain most involved in creating implicit memories is the basal ganglia. The basal ganglia play a crucial role in procedural learning, habit formation, and motor memory. They help encode and store information related to skills, routines, and repetitive tasks that become automatic and unconscious over time.

References

13.2 The Central Nervous System – Anatomy and Physiology. (2013, March 6). Opentextbc.Ca. https://opentextbc.ca/anatomyandphysiology/chapter/13-2-the-central-nervous-system/

Brooks, D.N.; Baddeley, A.D. (1976). What can amnesic patients learn. Neuropsychologia, 14 (1), 111–129. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(76)90012-9

Bullemer, P.; Nissen, MJ.; Willingham, D.B. (1989). On the Development of Procedural Knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 15 (6): 1047–1060. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1047.

Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–209.

Dew, I. T. Z., & Cabeza, R. (2011). The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: behavioral and neural evidence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 174–190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05946.x

Ell, Shawn; Zilioli, Monica (2012), Categorical Learning, in Seel, Norbert M. (ed.), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, Springer US, pp. 509–512, doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_98, ISBN 978-1-4419-1428-6

Squire, L.R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 82(3), 171–177. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.319.8326. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005. PMID 15464402.

Squire, L. R. (2009). The Legacy of Patient H.M. for Neuroscience. Neuron, 61, 6–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.023

Squire, L. R., & Dede, A. J. O. (2015). Conscious and unconscious memory systems. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 5 (1). https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021667

Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of Memory, (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic Press.

Ullman, MT (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92 (1–2), 231–70. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008. PMID 15037131. S2CID 14611894.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Saul Mcleod, PhD

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.


Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

Ayesh Perera

Researcher

B.A, MTS, Harvard University

Ayesh Perera, a Harvard graduate, has worked as a researcher in psychology and neuroscience under Dr. Kevin Majeres at Harvard Medical School.

.content-unmask .mv-ad-box { display:none; } #printfriendly { line-height: 1.7; } #printfriendly #pf-title { font-size: 40px; }